Nap was and perhaps still is the most prominent and well-known anthropologist of our time and he has been for decades. As I said yesterday, I have not read his book, but was commenting on the issues and controversy, and reacting to what is being said in the high-visibility reviews (such as the New York Times on Sunday -- and again yesterday) and news stories his book has occasioned (again, the New York Times on Sunday, Inside Higher Education on Monday, The Chronicle of Higher Ed last week). And, who in Anthropology doesn't know the saga of the Tierney book (a long discussion of which, and defense of Chagnon, is here, e.g.), or the documentary about this ongoing food fight, and on and on?
The reason we are commenting on this is because it involves, and reflects, fundamental and heated disagreements within anthropology that are inherently part of the politics of the whole scene. I know of few if any who can remain even remotely neutral in regard either to the way Nap has been treated by the press or by his opponents within anthropology, or about the substantive issues involved.
The actual scientific issue has to do, essentially, with genetic determinism and the degree to which our behavioral, and consequently our physical traits are the product of natural selection. Everyone, with or (mainly) without any real knowledge of the data or even the issues, is chiming in, including pop-sci authors from various disciplines who are credited with relevant 'expertise' by journalists who need someone to interview.
Some of the reaction to Chagnon's work has to do with the harmful experiences the Yanomami have had at the hands of anthropologists and other scientists, missionaries, explorers, and exploiters from the outside world. This is about social politics related to views of imperialism, exploitation, and the like, not about the anthropology of the group themselves.
But much of the fire deals with the degree to which the Yanomami represent a 'primitive people' (as a famous Science paper by Jim Neel described them in 1970), implying not just that they didn't have cars and televisions, but that they constituted an archetype of our ancestral state, during which our nature evolved via Darwinian natural selection. Even as post-docs in his lab, we felt that Neel's title was culpable hyperbole for getting a major publication, which someone of Neel's stature could do (and we told him so).
The idea was that if they are an archetype of the ur-human, Alley Oop in the flesh, then studying them can be used to extrapolate our societal and behavioral nature and their origins into our evolutionary past. Specifically, and most controversially, do we live a Hobbesian life in which male violence and dominance hierarchies determine who reproduces, and violence is largely about capturing women? Is our behavior based on a history of a species engaged in relentless, winner-take-all striving to spread our genes? Does it explain (or justify) warfare, or the sexual seizure of women?
By designating them as 'the fierce people' in the title of his most famous book, which was the monograph on 'primitive' people for many years, Nap essentially made such an argument, as did others in the research group, prominently including Neel who was the leader of the medical team involved in the classical Yanomami studies. Neel had his own biomedical genetic reasons for accepting the head-man theory of life; reproduction highly concentrated in one or a few males provided a way to understand the amount of harmful recessive genetic variation that our species had evolved to carry at any given time, as a comparison to large, modern societies exposed to chemicals and radiation, which cause genetic mutations and could heavily increase our burden of harmful variation.
The core issues
We need not here question the ethics, tactics, or descriptions of the Yanomami, nor the data that were collected. These issues have been debated and disputed, but that is beyond our point. Instead, we can question how 'primitive' or unacculturated or uncontacted they were, because those facts affect the degree to which they were archetypal as representatives of our evolutionary past.
First, this area had known missionaries for a long time. Villages were sometimes located near mission stations, and tribesmen from other villages knew about and interacted with those 'mission' villages. The Venezuelan and/or Brazilian government had, we believe, been sending vaccination teams upriver through much of the area to vaccinate natives against smallpox. By the early 1800s European settlers had established homesteads here and there along some if not much of the Amazon system. Some of these had taken Yanomami wives and were raising families on small holdings along rivers and tributaries.
von Humboldt, 1806 |
As a result, and at the very least, even villages without known or remembered direct exposure, that were contacted in the 20th century, cannot be said to have been totally unaffected by the outside 'civilized' world. The degree to which this changed their culture was assumed by anthropologists to have been minimal, relative to the peoples of the outside world. But this was an assumption and, at least, far from complete.
Beyond this, the Yanomami practiced slash-and-burn agriculture. Whatever the evolutionary source of their agricultural knowledge, they were not 'hunter-gatherers', and they were tropical forest rather than open-country people, that is, they were not living in the generally assumed ecology of our species' ancestry. They were sparsely settled at the time of most western contact, but there are archeological and even some historical works by early explorers who reported what seem to be rather large, or even permanent, urban settlements.
It's important to realize that the Yanomami have not been represented as showing just some local, currently evolving traits, but as a showcase of human evolution generally. But their way of life is not how our ancestors evolved! For by far the majority of our evolution as a species, we evolved, as far as we know, as various sorts of pre-agricultural hunter-gatherers in tiny, very sparsely dispersed populations, in diverse ecosystems, mainly in the Old World and only partly in tropical rainforest conditions. Unless agriculture has no effect whatever on social structure and behavior, a rather dubious assumption, the Yanomami are not unexceptionable archetypes of the way natural selection molded our behavior.
Indeed, other anthropologists and observers have not even seen the Yanomami as being particularly 'fierce' or warlike. One who had been part of the studies told me, way back at the time of the most intense studies in the early '70s, that the Yanomami were the most 'pacific' (his word) population in the region. And other scientists working in the same area at the same time as Nap characterized them basically in benign terms. Each anthropologist his or her own biases and predispositions to see his/her own sociopolitical views in the 'other', a fact that led to deep and angry splits within anthropology, regarding whether we could ever be even close to 'objective' about such things (the highly inflammatory and destructive 'post-modernism' disputes).
But the really important points don't require that we deny the objective facts, or the Yanomami's violence and competitiveness, or their headman-based social structures, all of which may well be quite as Chagnon described them over many years of observation--even if that is what he preferentially saw, what appealed to him, or even if his visits triggered some of that mayhem, as has been alleged. To deny that they had violent sports or did raids and captured women or had domineering male leaders, is simply silly if not scurrilous, and this must be said of many of the critics of Nap's work who often have been self-serving demagogues, to put it mildly.
Extrapolation is not needed to see the issues
We need not guess about whether the Yanomami social behavior represents human existence in our primeval past, because anthropologists have identified living hunter-gatherers and swidden agriculturalists with a wide range of social structures. The degree to which they had male dominance associated with substantially greater reproductive success, or had murderous rapine violence, are debated (or hotly debated). But there is a lot of variation, and violence and inequity are not the predominant characteristics reported for hunter-gatherer societies, even if they indubitably had their occasional heated disputes, as do we all. Indeed, to my recollection, studies of the surrounding South American indigenous groups do not characterize them as inherently or particularly violent. The Yanomami data themselves do not support the kind of genetic interpretation of head-man fitness advantage.
But even were we to accept Nap's data and inferences, there is another plausible reason for the violent and territory based tendencies he reports, a reason not at all based on intemperate inherency. The late 19th and early 20th centuries were ones during which outside explorers and exploiters pushed into much of the Amazon basin. Among the major aspects of that was the rubber trade. Rubber traders exploited the Indians badly in the usual ways, coming up the Amazon and then into its branching tributaries, and the fleeing Indians, who hadn't been enslaved or killed for target practice (yes, literally) may well have put frenzied territorial pressures on the Yanomami a century ago with consequences for their way of life when Chagnon and others first visited them. But of course it would in that case be a recent, rather than inherent cultural trait. There was indirect evidence for this in some of the demographic data in Nap's reports.
Such incursive pressures could plausibly have increased their wariness and warring behavior as a natural self-defense. Again, that would be a fact, but not a necessary aspect, of Amazonian culture, and certainly not something that could be extrapolated back for the past thousands of generations, on other continents, during which our entire species' inherent 'tendencies', if we have any, were molded by natural selection, or to the extent that such molding even occurred. Such extrapolations are a reflection of assumptions or predilections of the extrapolator.
Thus, another important debate, and what really underlies some of the vitriol surrounding Chagnon and his fights with the less than noble savages in anthropology, is the degree to which there can be objectivity in observing cultures other than our own (or, perhaps, even including our own). Indeed, the total (and totally predictable) prior-commitment manifest by the many commenters on this issue, who may typically have minimal direct knowledge of the Yanomami or the technical issues involved, is probably in itself a case study in post-modernism!
Ötzi the ice man |
So even if behavioral Darwinism be a valid way to view our genomic evolution, which is not at all obvious, the Yanomami are not, like Ötzi, the Tyrolian neolithic iceman, a frozen instance of a unitary distant past.
So, sociobiology or sociobalderdash?
The point of this post is not to attempt to adjudicate, but to point out that the issues underlying the heat of the dispute have to do with both the truth of the Darwinian interpretation of human behavioral nature, and the way that Dr Chagnon did, and reported, his studies in the Yanomami. The extent of genetic determinism is a legitimate one, and we must also recognize the wishful-thinking among anthropologists--on both sides of the arguments--that leads them to see what they see, and undoubtedly colors what and how they report it.
To repeat: Much more important is the degree to which observations today can be credibly extrapolated into the past, from one part of the world to all of humanity's patrimony. All of this ado over Nap's work is irrelevant to that question: Even were his descriptions indisputably 100% accurate, they don't contribute to the greater legitimate debate about the nature of our evolution. Yanomami culture today, in the Amazon, says nothing about our African past 200,000 years ago.
One way to see the colorful charivari that has always surrounded Dr Chagnon has to do with the knowledge of his nature, not just the nature of his knowledge.
Hi Ken, thank you for this very important post. It is a *must read* for anyone who has ever heard of Napoleon Chagnon. My question is why he ever thought--as you demonstrate so well--that a group of people practicing slash-and-burn horticulture with steel axes were some sort of window into human nature or even human evolutionary history. Did he miss the "Man the Hunter" conference with Richard Lee, Marshall Sahlins, et al.? Weren't these the very issues on the table in the 1960s?
ReplyDeleteThanks, Jason.
DeleteNap would have been well aware of Man the Hunter and many other things circulating at the time. Sahlins had his own axe to grind (e.g., in 'The Original Affluent Society'), and Lee himself I think anticipated Pinker (or the latter cribbed from the former) claiming that the San had as high a murder rate (and fights, as I recall, were over women) as Detroit or something. And extended that to make it 'the' human Darwinian story.
This was all 'in the air' at the time, in popular press as well as among anthropologists and 'ethologists' at the time. Feminism got into the act with their particular take. We all ate up the stories of primate and other animal dominance hierarchies and the like.
I think one could probably show that it was this that, in part, led to the post-modern movement, a backlash in protest to this hyper-theorizing.
So it was not unusual for Nap and Neel to see the violence in their particular Darwinian light. Others, including some studying the Yanomami, saw quite the opposite as I noted in my post.
The Darwinization of behavior and its opponents had deep, almost 'religious' beliefs about what they saw and/or wanted to see. The subsequent book by Wilson, who I think coined the term 'sociobiology' a few years later, along with Hamilton's theory of altruism's evolution, fueled the fire.
Most of the authors couldn't resist the temptation to extend all of this to humans, and the press ate it up.
To me it is the often uncritical, un-nuanced, and categorical tenor of these various views that do a disservice to any sense of science here. And, of course, I have my own views, and they too have a political component.
For example, uncritical Darwinian views of behavior, even after the eugenics and Nazi era, simply won't go away even if, no matter the degree to which they are, or aren't cogent, and even though we know of the societal damage they can be exploited to lead to. So these can't be seen as neutral
To defend Neel and perhaps Chagnon (I never discussed this part with him), besides this being in the air at the time, if the Yanomami represented 'primitive' (aboriginal) human culture in the demographic sense of having unequal male reproduction, some sort of head or chief kinds of roles, and were small with classificatory kinship structures, etc., then from the point of view of genetic variability--Neel's main interest--it might not matter whether the specific form of violence represented an aboriginal state. If it had the genetic effect, that would be enough if you held to a rather simplified view of natural selection.
Of course, the idea of this sort of dominance and its consequences for reproduction and hence Darwinian fitness can be gerrymandered to fit what one wants to fit and hence almost any society one studies. Even if the social dominance structure exists, there are many reasons--well known by now--why they need not lead to the kinds of genetic consequences suggested, and that is what must hold if one wants to make an evolutionary story of them.
Hi Ken, thank you for this elaboration. I guess I naively imagined, given what was up for discussion, that it would have been more obvious how a slash-and-burn horticulture population would not be a good window onto pre-agriculture. Perhaps not!
Deletedoesn't north australian descriptions of mother in law bestowal and the dominant position of the mother in law's mother in the whole exchange system discussed by levi-strauss, the mother in law bestowal described by goodall and by shapiro sort of make the whole idea of the big man model rather moot?
ReplyDeleteat the same time mother in law bestowal insures gerontocracy [gerondogamy ?] on the male side, it insures the decision making roles of senior females. together with a whole subculture of extra-marital liason.
a half a century later and all of this argument goes on about one person and his work where there are other descriptions of far more "primitive" primitives which show very very different societies.
the key fact that makes humans different is exchange, economy. no human consumes their own production exclusively. the human species evolved based on exchange economies, contractual agreements between individuals and groups so that every individual eventually participates.
not hunting, not fighting, not male dominance but exchange. without that system of economic exchange nobody benefits. and in "primitive" societies essentially nobody controls means of production. in fact controlling the means of production would greatly diminish it. everybody produces or everybody loses.
it all looks so damned silly to me reading the same arguments about who has the biggest one, a half a century after i read anthropology 101. that simply isn't how it works. alpha males may rule on wall street but it isn't how we evolved. it reads like we're still fighting over 19th century theories of patriarchy. life is not a rugby match.
Another cogent and thoughtful reply. Thanks!
ReplyDeleteCertainly it looks relentless (or relentlessly silly), but in fact behavioral Darwinism of the same sort is widespread among new investigators, and has been de rigueur since Darwin's own time (and, to some extent, in Darwin's Descent of Man).
The people holding these views are not stupid or ignorant, and if they are committed to the views, they have reasons. I don't happen to share the view, but I have no lock on the truth.
However, the issues warrant some more putting in context, and too much for a reply to a comment, so I'll try to add another post in the series to respond.
I definitely appreciate your writing here after seeking out the reviews of several cultural anthropologists and being shocked at their self-righteousness and blatant dishonesty. Thanks for the background.
ReplyDeleteHaving just finished "Noble Savages", along with Chagnon's (in)famous science paper, though, I worry that you're straw manning his ideas. Do you have any quotes to the effect that he believed or believes the Yanomamo represent some sort of exact replica of our ancestors? That just seems too silly for anyone to believe.
Obviously, though, it's one thing to say foraging or tribal peoples aren't exact replicas or "living fossils," and another thing entirely to claim (I think we agree, wrongly) that their lifestyles are completely irrelevant to discussions of the evolution of human behavior.
No scientist is foolish enough to make categorical statements. So one can always find caveats. Neel knew the Yanomami weren't identical to all human ancestral populations, and I don't remember what caveats (other than using quotes in the title) are in his famous Science 'Lessons' paper. But the idea he had, and I think Nap as well, was that the general point of male competition and dominance (for example) was a general characteristic of human ancestral populations.
DeleteSo in that sense the Y (and their Y chromosomes!) were being held up as a model of that sort--one might say in a sense similar to using mouse models for human disease: very informative even if not perfect.
I know from working with him that Neel clearly had this general idea in mind. He was interested in how human social structure _in our species' past_ had patterned the amount of standing genetic variation in the population. He wanted to compare 'primitive' peoples to those in huge industrial nations, in regard to the load of harmful mutations.
So, while not unaware of the imperfection of the model he certainly knew of these things. In these posts I comment on a possible reason for the Y's aggressiveness (if one were to accept that they were in fact particularly aggressive) having to do with recent effects of the Amazonian rubber trade.
If that were important, then even if the dominant-male advantage were true today, it would say nothing about our variation as a species. Not good if one wants to use them as a model for the latter.
I pointed this out to Neel when I worked on Yanomami demography with him. But I don't remember our discussions on how this would affect his views.